A Grammar of German Sign Language (DGS)

3.4.1. Types of relative clause

DGS belongs to the class of languages which use postnominal head-internal relative clauses. The relative clause follows the head noun it modifies and it is introduced by a relative pronoun occupying the sentence-initial position. This is illustrated by the following example: the head noun book precedes the relative clause ix( H) poss1 father read. The relative pronoun ix(H) [Lexicon 3.7.6] appears in sentence-initial position. In addition, the relative pronoun is accompanied by a non-manual marker, typically raised eyebrows (on non-manuals, see [Syntax 3.4.6]).

 

                                                 re

         [ book (ix3) [ ix(H)3 poss1 father read ]CP ]DP

         โ€˜the book which my father is readingโ€™

      

(based on Pfau & Steinbach, 2005: 512)

 

Relative clauses can modify subjects and objects of the matrix clause. In addition, the relative pronoun itself can receive different grammatical functions in the relative clause. We find the following combinations of grammatical role assignment:

 

a.       head noun: subject        relative pronoun: subject

b.       head noun: subject        relative pronoun: object

c.       head noun: object          relative pronoun: subject

d.       head noun: object          relative pronoun: object

 

Irrespective of the grammatical function the relative pronoun receives, it always occupies the sentence-initial position of the relative clause. In this respect, relative pronouns equal topics that also occupy the sentence-initial position at the left periphery (see [Syntax 2.3.3.3]). Similar to topicalization, this movement to the sentence-initial (topic) position is typically marked by the non-manual marker raised eyebrows as illustrated in the example above (see [Pragmatics 4.2]).

List of editors

Sina Proske, Derya Nuhbalaoglu, Annika Herrmann, Jana Hosemann & Markus Steinbach

Copyright info

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 License.
For details go to https://creativecommons.org/share-your-work/licensing-examples/#by-nc-sa

Bibliographical reference for citation

The entire grammar:
Branchini, Chiara and Lara Mantovan (eds.). 2020. A Grammar of Italian Sign Language (LIS). 1st ed. (SIGN-HUB Sign Language Grammar Series). (http://sign-hub.eu/grammars/...) (Accessed 31-10-2021)

A Chapter:
Smith, Mary. 2020. Syntax: 3. Coordination and Subordination. In Branchini, Chiara and Lara Mantovan (eds.), A Grammar of Italian Sign Language (LIS). 1st ed. (SIGN-HUB Sign Language Grammar Series), 230-237. ((http://sign-hub.eu/grammars/...) (Accessed 31-10-2021)

A Section:
Smith, Mary. 2020. Phonology: 1.1.1.2. Finger configuration. In Mary, Smith, Ben Smith and Carlo Smith (eds.), A Grammar of Catalan Sign Language (LSC). 1st ed. (SIGN-HUB Sign Language Grammar Series), 230-237. (http://sign-hub.eu/grammars/...) (Accessed 31-10-2021)

Smith, Mary. 2020. Syntax: 3.1.2.1.3. Manual markers in disjunctive coordination. In Mary, Smith, Ben Smith and Carlo Smith (eds.), A Grammar of Catalan Sign Language (LSC). 1st edn. (SIGN-HUB Sign Language Grammar Series), 230-237. (http://sign-hub.eu/grammars/...) (Accessed 31-10-2021)

europe-flagThis project has received funding from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant Agreement No 693349.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike4.0 License.